|
Indian Pediatr 2009;46: 137-143 |
 |
Impact of Iron-Folic Acid Supplementation on
Cognitive Abilities of School Girls in Vadodara |
Aditi Sen and Shubhada J Kanani
From Department of Foods and Nutrition, Maharaja
Sayajirao University of Baroda, Vadodara, India.
Correspondence to: Dr Shubhada Kanani, 14, Anupam
Society, Behind Pizza Inn, Jetalpur Road,
Vadodara, India. E-mail:
[email protected]
Manuscript received: June 4, 2007;
Initial review completed: July 24, 2007;
Revision accepted: May 1, 2008. |
Abstract
Objectives: To assess impact of daily and
intermittent iron-folate (IFA) supplementation on cognition of
underprivileged primary schoolgirls in Vadodara.
Design: Experimental-control longitudinal
study.
Setting: Municipal primary schools.
Participants: Schoolgirls (n=161) in
the age group of 9 – 13 years.
Intervention: Participants at three randomly
selected schools were given IFA tablets (100 mg elemental iron + 0.5 mg
folic acid) either once weekly or twice weekly or daily for one year. The
fourth was the control school.
Outcome measures: Digit span, maze test,
visual memory test, and clerical task scores.
Results: IFA supplementation given daily and
twice-weekly significantly improved cognition in most tests; the effect
was not seen in once-weekly or control groups. In daily and twice weekly
IFA groups, positive change in cognition test scores was relatively higher
in girls with good compliance(>70% dose) vs. poor compliance; in anemic
(hemoglobin<11 g/dL) vs non-anemic girls and in those with higher
hemoglobin (Hb) gain (>1g/dL) vs. lower Hb gain.
Conclusion: Twice weekly IFA supplementation
is comparable to daily IFA in terms of beneficial effects on cognition in
young adolescent girls.
Keywords: Anemia, Cognition, Iron-folate supplemen-tation, School
girls. |
Iron deficiency in school age and early
adolescence has been primarily studied for its detrimental effect on
hematinic status. However, in iron deficiency, decreased brain iron stores
may impair the activity of iron-dependent enzymes necessary for the
synthesis, function, and degradation of neurotransmitters(1) and produce
scholastic under-achievement or behavioral disturbances in school
children(2,3). Iron thus plays a vital role in cognitive development of
growing children and adolescents. Studies indicate that iron
supplementation has positive effects on hemoglobin (Hb) levels and
growth(4,5). It may also have beneficial impact on cognitive abilities, in
young women(6). It is worthwhile to explore if this impact will be seen
with intermittent iron-folate supplements in young adolescents entering
puberty. As suggested by the mucosal block theory, weekly or intermittent
iron supplementation may show overall similar iron absorption over the
week as compared to daily supplements. Further, intermittent doses reduce
side effects, increase compliance, and reduce cost. Once weekly
(iron-folic acid) IFA has already shown significant impact on Hb levels
among adolescent girls, which were comparable to those with daily
supplements(7). Since little is known about other non-hematinic benefits
of intermittent and daily IFA supplementation, this study was undertaken
with the objective of assessing the impact of intermittent (once and twice
weekly) and daily IFA supplementation on cognitive abilities of
underprivileged schoolgirls in early adolescence (9-13 years).
Methods
This was an experimental-control semi-longitudinal
study; an efficacy trial to assess impact of iron-folic acid supplements
on cognition.
Sampling: Using accepted procedures, desired sample
size was calculated(8), which came to 46 per group. Allowing for dropouts,
each study group required about 60 subjects of age 9-13 years; which were
available in standards V and VI per school. Thus, four schools were
randomly sampled from the sampling universe of 17 schools (all municipal
primary schools for girls in the morning shift), and all the consenting
girls in Standards V-VI were enrolled. The schools were comparable as
regards variables which could independently influence cognition; such as
home enviromnent (socio-economic background; slum conditions; parents’
education) and school environment (government schools with similar syllabi
and teaching facilities). Prior permission from the Primary School Board,
Vadodara and informed consent from the students and their parents were
taken.
Intervention: Three schools were randomly decided
as experimental schools (ES) and the fourth was the control school (CS)(n=65).
The participants in the three ES schools were given IFA tablets (100 mg
elemental iron + 0.5 mg folic acid): either once weekly (E1: IFA-1Wkly) (n=94)
or twice weekly (E2: IFA-2Wkly) (n=118) or daily (ED: IFA-Daily) (n=81)
for one year. The investigators maintained regular supply of IFA,
supervised the distribution and recorded compliance in all the schools.
The class monitor/class teacher assisted and maintained compliance
registers.
Pre-and post-intervention hemoglobin data were
collected on all girls. In view of limited working school days, and the
time required to conduct four cognitive tests twice in a year; a random
60% sample (n=240) was selected for the tests of cognition. Of these, data
of 161 girls was available pre and post intervention; after also excluding
girls who had attained menarche prior to or during the study, though they
did receive IFA supplements.
Outcome variables: Hemoglobin levels were measured
using cyanmethemoglobin method(9). The cognitive functions of the girls
were assessed using two tests (Digit span and Maze test) adapted from the
Gujarati version of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)(10)
and two tests earlier used in the department; suitably modified for this
group. These tests have been employed in previous studies on school
children and adolescents and found to be valid(11). Digit Span assesses
shortterm memory for numbers, attention - concentration, and ability to
recall the correct sequence of the numbers backward and forward; Maze test
assesses visual-motor coordination and speed, and fine motor coordination;
Visual Memory test assesses short-term memory; Clerical task assesses the
ability to concentrate and discriminate (in this case locating similar
looking (but different) letters on a page of Gujarati text). The change in
each group for hemoglobin and cognition test scores (means and standard
deviations) pre and post-intervention was calculated and compared between
study groups. Girls with good compliance were defined as those who
consumed ³70%
of the tablets given. All the data were coded, entered and analyzed in Epi
Info, Version 6.04-d(12). To compare intervention groups for statistical
significance of impact (P<0.05), ANOVA test; and to compare each
group with control, students t test was used.
Results
At baseline, all the four groups were statistically
similar having comparable cognition test scores and Hb levels (P>0.05)
(Table I,II). Overall, the mean hemoglobin (Hb) was 11.3
g/dL at baseline, and more than two-third (68.3%) of the girls were anemic
(Hb <12 g/dL).
Table I
Change in Mean Hemoglobin (g/dL) Levels After The Intervention
Study
Groups |
N |
Initial |
Final |
Mean
|
E1 |
65 |
11.5 ± 1.06 |
12.1 ± 0.44 |
0.6 ± 0.88 |
E2 |
89 |
11.1 ± 1.47 |
12.0 ± 0.41 |
1.0 ± 1.23 |
ED |
59 |
11.3 ± 1.69 |
12.2 ± 0.55 |
0.9 ± 1.38 |
CS |
41 |
11.5 ± 0.67 |
11.6 ± 0.63 |
0.03 ± 0.24 |
E1: Once weekly IFA supplementation; E2:
twice weekly IFA supplementation; ED:
daily IFA supplementation;
CS: Control, no supplementation. Values are Mean±SD.
|
Table II
Change in Mean Cognitive Test Scores After the Intervention
Study Groups |
N |
DS Test |
Clerical Task |
VMT |
Maze Test |
IFA-1Wkly (E1) |
43 |
1.05 ± 1.47 |
0.15 ± 0.12 |
0.19 ± 0.13 |
3.25 ± 3.39 |
IFA-2Wkly (E2) |
42 |
1.41 ± 1.28 |
0.34 ± 0.21 |
0.20 ± 0.13 |
4.35 ± 3.16 |
IFA-Daily (ED) |
42 |
2.56 ± 2.05 |
0.22 ± 0.14 |
0.21 ± 0.15 |
4.07 ± 3.24 |
No-IFA (CS) |
34 |
0.50 ± 1.23 |
0.14± 0.16 |
0.08 ± 0.14 |
1.47 ± 3.12 |
IFA-1Wkly (E1): Once weekly IFA supplementation;
IFA-2Wkly (E2): twice weekly IFA supplementation; IFA-Daily (ED): daily IFA supplementation;
No-IFA (CS): Control, no supplementation Comparing with control:
E2 and ED were highly significant (P<0.001) in all cognition tests except clerical task.
Within experimental group: In 2 of the 4 tests (DS and clerical task), ED significantly better than E2 (P<0.05);
E2 significantly better than E1 (P<0.001).
|
Impact on hemoglobin levels: Each of the
intervention groups showed significant higher (P<0.001) Hb
increment vs. the controls (Table I), with the mean Hb
increment the highest (1.0 g/dL) in the IFA-2Wkly Group. The mean Hb
increments among initially anemic girls in all the supplemented groups
were higher than those among initially non-anemic girls, with IFA-Daily
group showing highest increment (1.9 g/dL) followed closely by IFA-2Wkly
(1.6 g/dL).
Mean compliance with IFA tablets was 72%. Good
compliance ( ³70%
dose consumed) was seen in more than 50% subjects in all 3 groups. Mean
increase in Hb was significantly higher in good compliance group (0.9 g/dL
to 1.5 g/dL) than in poor compliance group (0.08 g/dL to 0.3 g/dL), with
impact better in IFA-Daily and IFA-2Wkly (P<0.001).
Impact on cognitive function: Experimental
subjects showed higher increments in test scores than controls (Table
II). On comparing each group with control by ‘t’ test, the increments
in IFA-Daily and IFA-2Wkly were significantly higher than No-IFA, in all
the four cognitive function (CF) tests; whereas IFA-1Wkly was
significantly higher, only in two of the four tests. Within the intervened
groups, in Digit Span and Clerical Task, IFA-Daily and IFA-2Wkly (p<0.05)
had significantly better scores than IFA-1Wkly. Overall, IFA-Daily and
IFA-2Wkly showed marked improvement in most tests, while IFA-1Wkly
consistently showed less improvement in cognitive test scores.
Girls with good compliance showed better improvement in
all four tests compared to those with poor compliance. IFA-2Wkly with good
compliance was comparable to IFA-Daily in three of the four tests. In
IFA-1wkly, even those with good compliance showed less impact than the
comparison groups in 2Wkly and IFA-Daily regimens.
Hemoglobin gain vs cognitive function: Fig.1
reveals that cognitive function scores were higher (though not signficant)
among those who gained more than 1 g/dL Hb than those who gained less (P>0.05).
Within those who gained higher level of Hb, IFA-2Wkly and IFA-Daily had
better improvement in scores than IFA-1Wkly.
Fig.2 depicts the mean change in cognitive
function test scores in initially anemic girls.
 |
 |
IFA-1Wkly: Once weekly IFA supplementation; IFA-2Wkly: twice weekly
IFA supplementation; IFA-Daily: daily IFA supplementation; No-IFA:
Control, no supplementation
Fig. 2 Mean change in cognitive function
test scores in initially anemic girls. |
Discussion
The findings of this study indicate that daily and
twice weekly iron folate supplementation are comparable as regards
significant impact on hemoglobin levels as well as cognitive functions of
girls in the pubertal phase of development. The once-weekly IFA group
consistently performed less satisfactorily in all the cognitive function
tests. Further, those intitially anemic and those showing higher Hb gain
(atleast 1 g/dL) showed better impact on cognitive function test scores;
again more clearly in the daily and twice-weekly dose regimens; with daily
showing best impact. This evidence indicates that a higher uptake of iron
(more frequent than once-weekly) is needed to lead to cognitive
improvements; and the encouraging finding is that twice weekly IFA was
consistently comparable to daily IFA in this regard. This finding has
important program implications as twice weekly IFA supplement will cost
less and be more feasible to deliver to beneficiary girls than daily IFA.
As regards the absorption of iron and reduction of
anemia from a physiological perspective, studies reviewed by Hallberg(13)
on daily as well as intermittent IFA supplementation reported that six
times more iron was absorbed when IFA was given daily than when given
weekly and concluded that there was no mucosal block during oral iron
therapy in humans. However, he further stated that, if relatively high
iron doses were given for a long time to subjects with low grade anemia,
then all the doses of iron, all dosage schedules, and all iron
preparations will give a similar Hb response. Thus, in the long run,
intermittent iron-folate therapy will perhaps have a satisfactory impact
on reducing anemia at lower cost and greater compliance. From our study
and from literature reviewed(4,14,15), it appears that a long-term
supplementation program, whether once or twice weekly, is likely to be as
effective as daily IFA with regard to improvements in hemoglobin levels.
Literature relating to functional benefits of IFA
interventions (such as cognitive abilities) among young adolescents is
limited. In a double blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial in
Baltimore, post intervention, non-anemic iron deficient adolescent girls
(N=73) (serum ferritin
£12
µg/L with normal Hb) receiving oral ferrous sulphate (650 mg twice daily)
performed better on a test of verbal learning and memory than the control
group (P<0.02)(16). Studies done in Indonesia concluded that daily
IFA supplementation to anemic school children significantly improved
learning-achievement scores after three months(17-19). In rural Varanasi,
6-8 year olds showed significant improvements in tests such as object
assembly and digit span after one year intervention (ferrous gluconate 200
mg)(20). We have also observed earlier a positive impact of IFA on
cognitive abilities of school children (9-15 years) in Vadodara, who were
supplemented with 60 mg elemental iron + 0.5 mg folic acid for 3 months
(unpublished data).
This study underscores the importance of reducing
anemia to improve cognition by showing that higher the magnitude of gain
in Hb, higher the gain in CF test scores. It further indicates that to
improve cognition, twice weekly IFA is superior to once weekly IFA and is
likely to show a similar impact as daily IFA supplementation. Supervised
supplementation, an important aspect of intermittent dosing, is feasible
in school settings with active participation of class monitors and
teachers.
Contributors: AS collected, analysed and
interpreted the data, performed literature review and drafted the
manuscript. SJK designed the study, supervised data collection, analysis.
She also revised and approved the final manuscript.
Funding: None.
Competing interests: None stated.
What is Already Known?
• Daily Iron-folate (IFA) supplementation
improves hematinic status and growth among adolescent girls.
What This study Adds?
• Intermittent IFA supplementation (twice-weekly)
was comparable to daily IFA as regards significantly improving
cognitive functions among school girls (9-13 years) and showed
better impact than once-weekly IFA. |
References
1. Erikson KM, Jones B, Beard JL. Altered functioning
of dopamine D 1 and D2
receptors in brains of iron deficient rats. Physiol Pharmacol Behav 2001;
69: 409-418.
2. Pollitt E, Liebel RL. Iron deficiency and behaviour.
J Pediatr 1976; 88: 372-381.
3. Sen A, Kanani SJ. Deleterious functional impact of
anemia on young adolescent school girls. Indian Pediatr 2006; 43 :
219-226.
4. Kanani S, Poojara RM. Supplementation with IFA
enhances growth in adolescent Indian girls. J Nutr 2000;130: 452S-455S.
5. Lawless JW, Latham MC, Stephenson LS, Kinoti SN,
Pertet AM. Iron supplementation improves appetite and growth in anemic
Kenyan primary school children. J Nutr 1994; 124: 645-654.
6. Murray-Kolb L, Beard JL. Iron treatment normalizes
cognitive functioning in young women. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 85: 778-787.
7. Beard JL. Weekly iron intervention: The case from
intermittent iron supplementation. Am J Clin Nutr 1998; 68: 209-212.
8. Fisher AA, Laing J, Stoeckel J, Townsend JW.
Handbook for Family Planning, Operations Research Design. 2nd edition. New
York: The Population Council; 1991.
9. International Nutritional Anemia Consultancy Group.
Guidelines for Eradication of Iron Deficiency Anemia. A Report of
International Nutritional Anemia Consultancy Group. New York and
Washington DC: INACG: The Nutrition Foundation; 1977.
10. Bhatt M. Gujarati adaptation of Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children. Ahmedabad-India: Jayshree Mudranalaya
Press; 1973.
11. Kanani S, Singh P, Zutshi R. The impact of daily
iron vs. calcium supplementation on growth, physical work capacity and
mental functions of school going adolescent boys and girls (9 to16 yrs) of
Vadodara. MD Thesis,Department of Foods and Nutrition, M. S. University of
Baroda, Vadodara, India. 1999.
12. Epi Info, Version 6.04-d. Centre for Disease
Control and Prevention (CDC). Epidemiology Program Office, Atlanta,
Georgia and World Health Organization, Global Programme on AIDS. Geneva:
Switzerland; 2001.
13. Hallberg Z. Combating iron deficiency: daily
administration of iron is far superior to weekly administration. Am J Clin
Nutr 1998; 68: 213-217.
14. Kotecha PV, Patel RZ, Karkar PD, Nirupam S. Impact
evaluation of Adolescent Girls’ Anemia Reduction Program Vadodara
district. Department of Preventive and Social Medicine. Government Medical
College, Government of Gujarat and Unicef – Gujarat. 2002.
15. Sharma A, Prasad K, Rao KV. Identification of an
appropriate strategy to control anemia in adolescent girls of poor
communities. Indian Pediatr 2000; 37: 261-267.
16. Bruner AB, Joffe A, Duggan K, Casella JF, Brandt J.
Randomized study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in
non-anemic iron deficient girls. Lancet 1996; 348: 992-996.
17. Soemantri AG, Pollitt E, Kim I. Iron deficiency
anemia and educational achievement. Am J Clin Nutr 1985; 42:1221-1228.
18. Soemantri AG. Preliminary findings on iron
supplementation and learning achievement of rural Indonesian children. Am
J Clin Nutr 1989; 50: 698-702.
19. Soewonda S, Husaini M, Pollitt E. Effects of iron
deficiency on attention and learning processes in preschool children:
Bandung, Indonesia. Am J Clin Nutr 1989; 50: 667-674.
20. Agarwal DK, Upadhyay SK, Tripathi AM, Agarwal KN. Nutritional
Status, Physical Work Capacity and Mental Function in School Children. New
Delhi: Scientific report 6, Nutrition Foundation of India; 1987. |
|
 |
|